Elsevier

Sleep Medicine

Volume 16, Issue 7, July 2015, Pages 862-870
Sleep Medicine

Original Article
Emotional memory processing is influenced by sleep quality

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2015.01.024Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Low sleep quality and lack of sleep both negatively affect emotional valence of memories.

  • The affective tone of the emotional memory is preserved after a good sleep.

  • Sleep deprivation leads to a decreased accuracy of emotional memory recognition.

  • The poor sleepers and the good sleepers have preserved sleep-dependent consolidation of emotional information.

Abstract

Objective

The recall of emotional memory is enhanced after sleep and is hindered by sleep deprivation. We used an emotional memory task to assess whether poor sleep quality, as well as sleep deprivation, may influence the accuracy of memory recognition, but also the affective tone associated with the memory.

Methods

Seventy-five subjects, divided into poor sleeper (PS), good sleeper (GS), and sleep deprivation (SD) groups, completed two recall (R) sessions: R1, 1 h after the encoding phase; and R2, after one night of sleep for PS and GS groups and after one night of sleep deprivation for the SD group. During the encoding phase, the participants rated valence and arousal of 90 pictures. During R1 and R2, the participants first made a yes/no memory judgment of the 45 target pictures intermingled with 30 non-target pictures, then rated valence and arousal of each picture.

Results

Recognition accuracy was higher for the PS and GS groups compared to the SD group for all pictures. Emotional valence of the remembered pictures was more negative after sleep deprivation and poor quality sleep, while it was preserved after a good sleep.

Conclusions

These results provide the first evidence that poor sleep quality negatively affects emotional valence of memories, within the context of preserved emotional memory consolidation. It is suggested that low sleep quality and lack of sleep may impose a more negative affective tone to memories. The reported effects are not to be ascribed to depressive mood, but to a specific influence of poor sleep quality.

Introduction

It is well known that emotional experiences have a privileged status in the memory for their affective resonance that contributes to boosting their long-term recall [1], [2]. The neuroanatomical explanation for this emotional memory enhancement effect is that emotional arousal activates the amygdala, which in turn influences other brain regions that are important for memory storage [3], [4]. In fact, the amygdala has reciprocal connections with the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex, and modulates the activity in the hippocampus–neocortical circuit during learning and memory consolidation [5].

In the last few years, it has been shown that sleep deprivation negatively influences the functionality of the above-mentioned brain circuits that are related to emotional memory. In particular, a reduced recall of emotional pictures in sleep-deprived subjects has been associated with a lack of reduction in amygdala reactivity when they were re-exposed to these same emotional stimuli at a recognition test administered 72 h [6] or six months after encoding [7]. Along the same line, Yoo et al., investigating the impact of 35 h of sleep deprivation on emotional reactivity, showed a greater activation of the amygdala and a reduced connectivity between the amygdala and the medial prefrontal cortex in response to negative images in sleep-deprived subjects, compared to the rested control group [8]. Therefore, lack of sleep affects the regulation of the cortico-limbic emotion circuits, negatively influences emotional memory consolidation and increases emotional reactivity.

According to the “sleep to forget and sleep to remember” hypothesis [9], those who do not benefit from sleep after learning are not able to decouple memories from their “affective blanket” during the post-learning period. Thus, the magnitude of the affective “charge” remaining within memory networks persists for a longer time, potentially resulting in a condition of chronic anxiety [10]. If this theory is correct, people with a poor sleep quality – similar to sleep-deprived subjects – should show an emotional memory consolidation impairment and an increase of the affective tone associated with the emotionally loaded stimuli or events.

To date, several studies have investigated the influence of sleep and sleep deprivation on emotional memory consolidation. The findings are generally consistent in indicating that sleep supports emotional memory consolidation [2], [11], [12], [13], [14], [15], [16], while sleep deprivation negatively affects it [2], [6], [7], [17], [18], [19], [20], [21].

However, it is believed that no study has yet examined the relationship between poor sleep quality and emotional memory in humans. The issue is of intrinsic clinical interest because there is growing evidence that sleep disturbances may constitute a risk factor in the development of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) [22] and depression [23], [24] by compromising sleep-dependent affective memory processing.

The Walker and van der Helm model holds that these psychiatric disorders may be caused and maintained by a poor sleep quality, which impairs the ability to form and retain memories of positive (and neutral) affective valence but leaves the long-term formation and the affective tone of negative experiences preserved [9].

Therefore, investigating the existence of a direct relationship between poor sleep quality and deficits in emotional memory processing may be important to prevent the development of psychiatric disturbances such as PTSD.

The main aim of the present study was to test the hypothesis that poor sleep quality, like sleep deprivation, is associated with a lower recall of emotional stimuli. Furthermore, the study assessed whether poor sleep quality and sleep deprivation may influence not only the accuracy of memory recognition (hippocampal activity-dependent) but also the affective tone associated with the memory (limbic activity-dependent). To this aim, a sample of subjects with poor sleep quality was compared with a sleep-deprived group and with a normally sleeping control group. A differential (d′) was considered as an index of performance accuracy (ie, a measure of accurate remembering) that should depend directly on the activity of the hippocampus, assuming that this index is a specific measure of declarative memory. On the other hand, both the valence and the arousal ratings were used as measures of the affective tone of a stimulus because it is known that they both depend on the limbic area activity [25], [26].

In keeping with the “sleep to forget and sleep to remember” hypothesis, the sleep-deprived group and, in a more attenuated form, the poor-sleep-quality group should show a lack of sleep-dependent modulation of the emotional memory, characterized by a decreased consolidation of the perceptual memory content and a failure in the reduction of its affective tone.

Section snippets

Participants

A sample of 100 undergraduate students completed the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) questionnaire [27]. From this sample, 91 subjects were selected to participate in the study.

Participants were classified, post hoc using the PSQI scores, as either good or poor sleepers (see below). The poor sleeper group (PS, n = 30) included students who had nocturnal sleep difficulties ≥3 nights a week and obtained a PSQI score >5. The good sleeper group (GS, n = 61) included students who reported no

Subjective sleepiness and mood

The ANOVA on ΔKSS scores in the recall 1 session did not show a significant main effect of Group (F2,72 = 0.60, p = 0.54). Instead, the ANOVA on ΔKSS scores in the recall 2 session was significant (F2,72 = 23.92, p = 0.0000001). Post hoc comparisons of the recall 2 sessions indicated that sleepiness was higher in the SD group compared to both the PS (p = 0.0000001) and GS groups (p = 0.00000001). Subjective levels of sleepiness were comparable in the recall 2 sessions for the PS group and GS

Discussion

In this study, it was demonstrated for the first time that individuals suffering from poor sleep have a preserved sleep-dependent consolidation of emotional information, showing the same post-sleep performance accuracy compared to good sleepers. However, interestingly, poor sleepers show a more negative evaluation of the neutral stimuli at recall.

On the other hand, a night of good sleep leads to an increase in memory consolidation of emotional stimuli, but leaves their affective valence

Conclusions

In conclusion, the present results show that if people do not sleep for a whole night, they do not consolidate emotional memory information, and that the little information that is remembered is enveloped in a negative valence. If people do not sleep well, they remember more events but, again, they are more negatively framed. These findings suggest a selective alteration in memory by which sleep-deprived individuals and poor sleepers tend to remember the experiences more negatively. This effect

Conflict of interest

The ICMJE Uniform Disclosure Form for Potential Conflicts of Interest associated with this article can be viewed by clicking on the following link: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2015.01.024.

. ICMJE Form for Disclosure of Potential Conflicts of Interest form.

Acknowledgement

We gratefully thank Sebastiano Pocchi for his help in data collecting.

References (58)

  • L. Kilpatrick et al.

    Amygdala modulation of parahippocampal and frontal regions during emotionally influenced memory storage

    Neuroimage

    (2003)
  • J.K. Olofsson et al.

    Affective picture processing: an integrative review of ERP findings

    Biol Psychol

    (2008)
  • P.L. Franzen et al.

    Sleep deprivation alters pupillary reactivity to emotional stimuli in healthy young adults

    Biol Psychol

    (2009)
  • C. Baglioni et al.

    Psychophysiological reactivity to sleep-related emotional stimuli in primary insomnia

    Behav Res Ther

    (2010)
  • D.M. Small et al.

    Dissociation of neural representation of intensity and affective valuation in human gustation

    Neuron

    (2003)
  • H. Fischer et al.

    Brain habituation during repeated exposure to fearful and neutral faces: a functional MRI study

    Brain Res Bull

    (2003)
  • C. Martin-Soelch et al.

    Anxiety trait modulates psychophysiological reactions, but not habituation processes related to affective auditory stimuli

    Int J Psychophysiol

    (2006)
  • M. Codispoti et al.

    Repetitive picture processing: autonomic and cortical correlates

    Brain Res

    (2006)
  • J.K. Gollan et al.

    Identifying differences in biased affective information processing in major depression

    Psychiatry Res

    (2008)
  • R.C. Gur et al.

    Facial emotion discrimination: II. Behavioral findings in depression

    Psychiatry Res

    (1992)
  • J.L. McGaugh et al.

    Involvement of the amygdala in memory storage: interaction with other brain systems

    Proc Natl Acad Sci USA

    (1996)
  • V. Sterpenich et al.

    Sleep-related hippocampo-cortical interplay during emotional memory recollection

    PLoS Biol

    (2007)
  • V. Sterpenich et al.

    Sleep promotes the neural reorganization of remote emotional memory

    J Neurosci

    (2009)
  • S.S. Yoo et al.

    The human emotional brain without sleep – a prefrontal amygdala disconnect

    Curr Biol

    (2007)
  • M.P. Walker et al.

    Overnight therapy? The role of sleep in emotional brain processing

    Psychol Bull

    (2009)
  • U. Wagner et al.

    Emotional memory formation is enhanced across sleep intervals with high amounts of rapid eye movement sleep

    Learn Mem

    (2001)
  • M. Nishida et al.

    REM sleep, prefrontal theta, and the consolidation of human emotional memory

    Cereb Cortex

    (2009)
  • J.M. Saletin et al.

    The role of sleep in directed forgetting and remembering of human memories

    Cereb Cortex

    (2011)
  • B. Baran et al.

    Processing of emotional reactivity and emotional memory over sleep

    J Neurosci

    (2012)
  • Cited by (59)

    • Changes in affect

      2023, Encyclopedia of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms: Volume 1-6, Second Edition
    • Association between snoring and depressive symptoms in adolescents

      2021, Journal of Psychiatric Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Sleep fragmentation and daytime sleepiness caused by sleep fragmentation are correlated with depressive symptoms (Sforza et al., 2002; Yue et al., 2003). Mechanistically, poor sleep quality due to snoring may impair emotional memory processing, thus imparting negative emotional valence to memories, in turn inducing depressive symptoms (Tempesta et al., 2015). Second, nightly intermittent hypoxemia by SDB can cause brain damage that may lead to depressive symptoms.

    • Sleep Loss and the Socio-Emotional Brain

      2020, Trends in Cognitive Sciences
      Citation Excerpt :

      Similar findings have been reported following selective rapid eye movement (REM) sleep deprivation rather than total sleep deprivation, suggesting a role for REM sleep in preserving high-resolution emotional mapping (see Outstanding Questions) [64]. Negative emotional bias has similarly been observed in adults after 3 weeks of in-laboratory partial sleep restriction (4 hours of sleep per night for 5 nights) [65], as well as in adults and adolescents suffering from poor sleep quality, as assessed using self-report and actigraphy measurements [66,67]. Contributing to this prejudicial aversive sensitivity may be attentional bias.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text