Elsevier

Sleep Medicine

Volume 10, Issue 3, March 2009, Pages 287-294
Sleep Medicine

Original Article
Phase advancing the human circadian clock with blue-enriched polychromatic light

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2008.05.005Get rights and content

Abstract

Background

Previous studies have shown that the human circadian system is maximally sensitive to short-wavelength (blue) light. Whether this sensitivity can be utilized to increase the size of phase shifts using light boxes and protocols designed for practical settings is not known. We assessed whether bright polychromatic lamps enriched in the short-wavelength portion of the visible light spectrum could produce larger phase advances than standard bright white lamps.

Methods

Twenty-two healthy young adults received either a bright white or bright blue-enriched 2-h phase advancing light pulse upon awakening on each of four treatment days. On the first treatment day the light pulse began 8 h after the dim light melatonin onset (DLMO), on average about 2 h before baseline wake time. On each subsequent day, light treatment began 1 h earlier than the previous day, and the sleep schedule was also advanced.

Results

Phase advances of the DLMO for the blue-enriched (92 ± 78 min, n = 12) and white groups (76 ± 45 min, n = 10) were not significantly different.

Conclusion

Bright blue-enriched polychromatic light is no more effective than standard bright light therapy for phase advancing circadian rhythms at commonly used therapeutic light levels.

Introduction

Scheduled exposure to light and darkness are effective tools for phase shifting the human circadian clock [1], [2]. Timed exposure to bright light is recognized by the American Academy of Sleep Medicine as an effective treatment for circadian rhythm sleep disorders, such as shift work disorder (SWD) and delayed sleep phase disorder (DSPD) [3], [4], [5]. Despite this recommendation, much research on the efficiency of light treatment remains to be done, to establish optimal parameters for the dosage (intensity and duration), timing and wavelength of light treatment in practical schedules that could be used in the home or workplace.

Evidence has recently emerged showing that circadian phase shifts in humans are most sensitive to short-wavelength light [6], [7], [8], [9], [10]. Although rod and cone photoreceptors contribute to non-image-forming behaviors such as phase shifting in animal models [11], [12], [13], and may also do so in humans [14], [15], [16], these responses appear primarily driven by a small population of intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells [17], [18], [19] containing the photopigment melanopsin [20], [21], [22], [23], [24]. The spectral sensitivity of non-image-forming (NIF) responses (e.g., the pupillary light reflex; light-induced melatonin suppression and circadian phase shifting) in humans was not known until 2001, when melatonin suppression was shown to be most sensitive to short-wavelength light [25], [26]. Consequently, most of the earlier circadian phase shifting studies using polychromatic light did not measure or did not report the amount of energy specifically in the blue portion of the visible light spectrum, but instead reported the illuminance of the light source, which typically ranged from 2000 to 12,000 lux [27], [28], [29], [30], [31], [32], [33]. Some specified the type of fluorescent lamp (e.g., “cool white” or “full spectrum”) or provided the correlated color temperature (CCT) [in ° kelvin (K)] of the lamps, which is a metric describing the relative proportion of warm versus cool colors in a light source. Most earlier studies used lamps with a CCT <7000 K because lamps with a higher CCT (containing more short-wavelength energy) were not readily available until recently. In the current study we used fluorescent lamps enriched with blue light, rated by the manufacturer as having a CCT of 17,000 K.

The spectral sensitivity of circadian phase shifting in humans has been assessed in carefully controlled studies that have used relatively dim, narrow bandwidth light administered with a specialized light delivery apparatus [6], [7], [8], [9], [10]. For example, one study pharmacologically dilated subject’s pupils, had subjects wear blackout goggles for 90 min prior to light exposure, and then administered a 6.5 h light pulse while subject’s heads were immobilized in a Ganzfeld dome [6]. Controls such as these are important for determining spectral sensitivity, but leave open the question of whether this sensitivity could be utilized in practical protocols to shift circadian rhythms, such as advancing rhythms before flying east to attenuate jet lag, or to treat a patient with DSPD. In addition, it is not known whether this sensitivity could be harnessed to increase the size of the phase shift relative to treatment with standard bright “white” light.

Lamps and light-producing devices emitting exclusively or relatively more short-wavelength energy are now commercially available [34]. This provides clinicians and patient/consumers with a variety of choices when selecting a device for light treatment, but there remains little evidence from well controlled studies demonstrating the efficacy of those devices for circadian phase shifting.

The goal of the current study was to determine whether bright blue-enriched light could phase advance the circadian clock more than standard bright white light at light levels that are currently being used for therapeutic applications, and using light boxes designed for practical applications.

Section snippets

Subjects

This was a between-subjects design in which subjects were randomly assigned to receive either white (n = 10) or blue-enriched (n = 12) light. The mean ± SD age (blue-enriched: 27 ± 7; white 28 ± 6), sex (blue-enriched: 7 M, 5 F; white: 6 M, 4 F) and morningness–eveningness [35] (blue-enriched: 55 ± 8; white 55 ± 9) of the groups was similar. Subjects did not report any medical, psychiatric, or sleep disorders as assessed by a telephone interview, an in-person interview, and several screening questionnaires.

Results

The average advance of wake time on the first treatment day (day 25), relative to each subject’s weekday wake time, was 2.2 ± 1.0 h for the blue-enriched group and 2.4 ± 0.8 h for the white group [t(20) = −0.46, p = 0.65]. The distribution of the advance of wake time on day 25 is also illustrated in Fig. 4.

Phase advances of the DLMO and DLMOff for the blue-enriched and white groups were not significantly different (DLMO 92 ± 78 vs. 76 ± 45 min; DLMOff 91 ± 95 vs. 61 ± 53 min, respectively) (Fig. 3).

There were

Discussion

We believe our data are the first comparing the effectiveness of different polychromatic lights for phase shifting human circadian rhythms. We found that the bright blue-enriched polychromatic light box did not produce larger phase advances of the circadian clock than the bright white light box at light levels commonly employed for therapeutic circadian phase shifting.

One other study has tested the impact of different polychromatic lights on human non-image-forming responses. In that study,

Acknowledgments

Supported by National Institutes of Health Grant R01 NR007677. Philips Lighting donated the blue-enriched light boxes. Enviro-Med donated the white light boxes. We thank Thomas Molina, Erin Cullnan, Heather Holly, Meredith Rathert, Jillian Canton, Daniel Alderson, Jonathan Swisher, Clara Lee, Meredith Durkin, Young Cho, Valerie Ellios, and Helen Burgess, Ph.D., for assistance with data collection. We thank Apollo Health for assistance with light measurements.

References (48)

  • V.L. Revell et al.

    Short-wavelength sensitivity of the human circadian system to phase-advancing light

    J. Biol. Rhythms

    (2005)
  • H.R. Wright et al.

    Effect of light wavelength on suppression and phase delay of the melatonin rhythm

    Chronobiol. Int.

    (2001)
  • H.R. Wright et al.

    Differential effects of light wavelength in phase advancing the melatonin rhythm

    J. Pineal Res.

    (2004)
  • N. Mrosovsky

    Contribution of classic photoreceptors to entrainment

    J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol.

    (2003)
  • N.F. Ruby et al.

    Role of melanopsin in circadian responses to light

    Science

    (2002)
  • M.S. Rea et al.

    Phototransduction for human melatonin suppression

    J. Pineal Res.

    (2002)
  • M.G. Figueiro et al.

    Preliminary evidence for spectral opponency in the suppression of melatonin by light in humans

    Neuroreport

    (2004)
  • V.L. Revell et al.

    Light-induced melatonin suppression in humans with polychromatic and monochromatic light

    Chronobiol. Int.

    (2007)
  • D.M. Dacey et al.

    Melanopsin-expressing ganglion cells in primate retina signal colour and irradiance and project to the LGN

    Nature

    (2005)
  • D.M. Berson et al.

    Phototransduction by retinal ganglion cells that set the circadian clock

    Science

    (2002)
  • S. Hattar et al.

    Melanopsin-containing retinal ganglion cells: architecture, projections, and intrinsic photosensitivity

    Science

    (2002)
  • X. Qiu et al.

    Induction of photosensitivity by heterologous expression of melanopsin

    Nature

    (2005)
  • S. Panda et al.

    Illumination of the melanopsin signaling pathway

    Science

    (2005)
  • Y. Fu et al.

    Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells detect light with a vitamin A-based photopigment, melanopsin

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

    (2005)
  • Cited by (99)

    • Light and melatonin treatment for jet lag

      2023, Encyclopedia of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms: Volume 1-6, Second Edition
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text